The relationship between far-right ideologies, devout religious adherence, and delusional thinking is multifaceted and can be understood through psychological, sociopolitical, and cultural dynamics. Research indicates that individuals who align with far-right beliefs often exhibit a propensity for conspiratorial thinking, which can be linked to delusional ideation. This connection is particularly evident in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, where conspiracy theories proliferated among far-right groups, suggesting a psychological framework where delusional thinking serves to reinforce in-group identities and justify out-group hostility Stoeckel & Ceka (2022)Sturm & Albrecht, 2020).
Far-right movements frequently capitalize on religious sentiments, intertwining them with nationalist ideologies. This fusion can lead to a heightened sense of existential threat, which may trigger delusional thinking as a coping mechanism. For instance, apocalyptic narratives prevalent in certain religious circles can resonate with far-right ideologies, creating a fertile ground for delusions that frame societal changes as existential crises (Sturm & Albrecht, 2020). The intertwining of religious fervor and extremist political beliefs can foster a worldview that is resistant to contradictory evidence, a hallmark of delusional thinking (Bronstein et al., 2019).
Moreover, studies have shown that individuals with low self-esteem are more susceptible to delusional ideation, particularly themes of persecution, which are often prevalent in far-right rhetoric (Warman & Lysaker, 2011; Warman et al., 2010). This suggests that the psychological profile of individuals drawn to far-right ideologies may predispose them to adopt delusional beliefs, particularly when these beliefs serve to validate their experiences of marginalization or victimization. The reinforcement of these beliefs through social networks and echo chambers further exacerbates the delusional mindset, as individuals are less likely to encounter dissenting viewpoints (Bronstein et al., 2019).
The role of dogmatism is also significant in this context. Research indicates that dogmatic individuals, including devout religious adherents, are more likely to endorse false beliefs and conspiracy theories, which can be seen as a form of delusional thinking (Bronstein et al., 2019). This dogmatism is often coupled with a cognitive style that favors intuitive over analytical thinking, making it challenging for individuals to critically assess their beliefs (Bronstein et al., 2019). The result is a reinforcement of delusional ideation that aligns with far-right narratives, which often portray a simplified, black-and-white view of complex social issues.
In summary, the interplay between far-right ideologies, devout religious beliefs, and delusional thinking is characterized by a shared reliance on conspiratorial narratives, a propensity for dogmatism, and psychological vulnerabilities such as low self-esteem. These factors collectively contribute to a worldview that is resistant to change and heavily influenced by in-group dynamics, ultimately fostering an environment where delusional thinking can thrive.
References
Arjmand, S., Kohlmeier, K., Behzadi, M., Ilaghi, M., Mazhari, S., & Shabani, M. (2020). Looking into a deluded brain through a neuroimaging lens. The Neuroscientist, 27(1), 73-87. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858420936172
Bronstein, M., Pennycook, G., Bear, A., Rand, D., & Cannon, T. (2019). Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking.. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(1), 108-117. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101832
Coltheart, M. (2010). The neuropsychology of delusions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191(1), 16-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05496.x
Gurin, L. and Blum, S. (2017). Delusions and the right hemisphere: a review of the case for the right hemisphere as a mediator of reality-based belief. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 29(3), 225-235. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16060118
Hayashi, N., Igarashi, Y., & Harima, H. (2021). Delusion progression process from the perspective of patients with psychoses: a descriptive study based on the primary delusion concept of karl jaspers. Plos One, 16(4), e0250766. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250766
Ismail, Z., Nguyen, M., Fischer, C., Schweizer, T., Mulsant, B., & Mamo, D. (2011). Neurobiology of delusions in alzheimer’s disease. Current Psychiatry Reports, 13(3), 211-218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-011-0195-1
Lai, L., Lee, P., Chan, P., Fok, M., Hsiung, G., & Sepehry, A. (2017). Prevalence of delusions in drug‐naïve alzheimer disease patients: a meta‐analysis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 34(9), 1287-1293. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4812
Mishara, A. and Fusar‐Poli, P. (2013). The phenomenology and neurobiology of delusion formation during psychosis onset: jaspers, truman symptoms, and aberrant salience. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(2), 278-286. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs155
Najjar, S., Pahlajani, S., Sanctis, V., Stern, J., Najjar, A., & Chong, D. (2017). Neurovascular unit dysfunction and blood–brain barrier hyperpermeability contribute to schizophrenia neurobiology: a theoretical integration of clinical and experimental evidence. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00083
Nomura, K., Kazui, H., Wada, T., Sugiyama, H., Yamamoto, D., Yoshiyama, K., … & Takeda, M. (2012). Classification of delusions in alzheimer’s disease and their neural correlates. Psychogeriatrics, 12(3), 200-210. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1479-8301.2012.00427.x
Perez, D., Pan, H., Weisholtz, D., Root, J., Tuescher, O., Fischer, D., … & Stern, E. (2015). Altered threat and safety neural processing linked to persecutory delusions in schizophrenia: a two-task fmri study. Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging, 233(3), 352-366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.06.002
Prasad, K. (2019). Delusions in alzheimer disease: what researchers should not forget. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27(5), 499-501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2018.12.034
Raij, T., Riekki, T., Rikandi, E., Mäntylä, T., Kieseppä, T., & Suvisaari, J. (2018). Activation of the motivation-related ventral striatum during delusional experience. Translational Psychiatry, 8(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0347-8
Stoeckel, F. and Ceka, B. (2022). Political tolerance in europe: the role of conspiratorial thinking and cosmopolitanism. European Journal of Political Research, 62(3), 699-722. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.12527
Sturm, T. and Albrecht, T. (2020). ‘constituent covid-19 apocalypses: contagious conspiracism, 5g, and viral vaccinations’. Anthropology and Medicine, 28(1), 122-139. https://doi.org/10.1080/13648470.2020.1833684
Sultzer, D., Leskin, L., Melrose, R., Harwood, D., Narvaez, T., Ando, T., … & Mandelkern, M. (2014). Neurobiology of delusions, memory, and insight in alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(11), 1346-1355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2013.06.005
Warman, D. and Lysaker, P. (2011). Delusional ideation and self-esteem in individuals with psychotic disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 199(1), 58-61. https://doi.org/10.1097/nmd.0b013e3182044b43
Warman, D., Lysaker, P., Luedtke, B., & Martin, J. (2010). Self-esteem and delusion proneness. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198(6), 455-457. https://doi.org/10.1097/nmd.0b013e3181e086c5